DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2
Hits 1 – 20 of 26

1
The Oxford handbook of negation
Deprez, Viviane (Hrsg.); Espinal, Teresa M. (Hrsg.). - Oxford : Oxford University Press, 2020
IDS Bibliografie zur deutschen Grammatik
Show details
2
Georgian Complex Segments
In: North East Linguistics Society (2020)
BASE
Show details
3
Pragmatic abilities in bilinguals ; Pragmatic abilities in bilinguals: The case of scalar implicatures
In: ISSN: 1879-9264 ; Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01803048 ; Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, John Benjamins Pub. Co., 2018, ⟨10.1075/lab.17017.dup⟩ (2018)
BASE
Show details
4
Relating (Un)acceptability to Interpretation. Experimental Investigations on Negation
BASE
Show details
5
English negative concord and double negation: The division of labor between syntax and pragmatics
In: Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America; Vol 3 (2018): Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America; 53:1–15 ; 2473-8689 (2018)
BASE
Show details
6
Action relevance in linguistic context drives word-induced motor activity.
In: ISSN: 1662-5161 ; Frontiers in Human Neuroscience ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01067818 ; Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Frontiers, 2014, 8, pp.163. ⟨10.3389/fnhum.2014.00163⟩ (2014)
Abstract: International audience ; Many neurocognitive studies on the role of motor structures in action-language processing have implicitly adopted a "dictionary-like" framework within which lexical meaning is constructed on the basis of an invariant set of semantic features. The debate has thus been centered on the question of whether motor activation is an integral part of the lexical semantics (embodied theories) or the result of a post-lexical construction of a situation model (disembodied theories). However, research in psycholinguistics show that lexical semantic processing and context-dependent meaning construction are narrowly integrated. An understanding of the role of motor structures in action-language processing might thus be better achieved by focusing on the linguistic contexts under which such structures are recruited. Here, we therefore analyzed online modulations of grip force while subjects listened to target words embedded in different linguistic contexts. When the target word was a hand action verb and when the sentence focused on that action (John signs the contract) an early increase of grip force was observed. No comparable increase was detected when the same word occurred in a context that shifted the focus toward the agent's mental state (John wants to sign the contract). There mere presence of an action word is thus not sufficient to trigger motor activation. Moreover, when the linguistic context set up a strong expectation for a hand action, a grip force increase was observed even when the tested word was a pseudo-verb. The presence of a known action word is thus not required to trigger motor activation. Importantly, however, the same linguistic contexts that sufficed to trigger motor activation with pseudo-verbs failed to trigger motor activation when the target words were verbs with no motor action reference. Context is thus not by itself sufficient to supersede an "incompatible" word meaning. We argue that motor structure activation is part of a dynamic process that integrates the lexical meaning potential of a term and the context in the online construction of a situation model, which is a crucial process for fluent and efficient online language comprehension.
Keyword: [SDV.NEU.SC]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Cognitive Sciences
URL: https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01067818/document
https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01067818
https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01067818/file/78373_Aravena_ProvisionalPDF.pdf
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00163
BASE
Hide details
7
Action relevance in linguistic context drives word-induced motor activity
Aravena, Pia; Courson, Mélody; Frak, Victor. - : Frontiers Media S.A., 2014
BASE
Show details
8
Syntax at hand: common syntactic structures for actions and language.
In: ISSN: 1932-6203 ; EISSN: 1932-6203 ; PLoS ONE ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00904143 ; PLoS ONE, Public Library of Science, 2013, 8 (8), pp.e72677. ⟨10.1371/journal.pone.0072677⟩ (2013)
BASE
Show details
9
The interaction of syntax, prosody, and discourse in licensing French wh-in-situ questions
In: ISSN: 0024-3841 ; Lingua ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01103338 ; Lingua, Elsevier, 2013, 124, Special Issue : SI Pages : 4-19. ⟨10.1016/j.lingua.2012.03.002⟩ (2013)
BASE
Show details
10
Syntax at Hand: Common Syntactic Structures for Actions and Language
Roy, Alice C.; Curie, Aurore; Nazir, Tatjana. - : Public Library of Science, 2013
BASE
Show details
11
Atoms of negation: An outside-in micro-parametric approach to negative concord
In: The Evolution of Negation: Beyond the Jespersen Cycle ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00925894 ; The Evolution of Negation: Beyond the Jespersen Cycle, Mouton de Gruyter, pp.221-272, 2012 (2012)
BASE
Show details
12
Grip force reveals the context sensitivity of language-induced motor activity during "action words" processing: evidence from sentential negation.
In: ISSN: 1932-6203 ; EISSN: 1932-6203 ; PLoS ONE ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00875165 ; PLoS ONE, Public Library of Science, 2012, 7 (12), pp.e50287. ⟨10.1371/journal.pone.0050287⟩ (2012)
BASE
Show details
13
Contextual effects on motor activation during "action word" processing: Grip force study of volition denoting sentences
In: The Neurobiology of Language Conference ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00932603 ; The Neurobiology of Language Conference, Oct 2012, San Sebastian, Spain (2012)
BASE
Show details
14
Which account of wh-/quantifier interaction should everyone adopt? A new take on a classic developmental puzzle
In: Proceedings of the 37th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development ; 37th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00932545 ; 37th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Nov 2012, Boston, United States. pp.1-12 (2012)
BASE
Show details
15
Tracking the rise of French Negative Quantifiers: a computational approach using ACOM
In: Association of the French Language ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00951700 ; Association of the French Language, Jun 2012, New Castle, United Kingdom (2012)
BASE
Show details
16
Grip Force Reveals the Context Sensitivity of Language-Induced Motor Activity during “Action Words
BASE
Show details
17
From N to negative D: charting the time course of the historical rise of French n-words
In: The Noun Phrase in Romance and Germanic: Structure, variation, and change ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00925595 ; Sleeman, Petra and Harry Perridon. The Noun Phrase in Romance and Germanic: Structure, variation, and change, John Benjamins Publishin Company, pp. 257-280, 2011 (2011)
BASE
Show details
18
Interfacing information and prosody
In: Romance languages and Linguistic Theory ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00927013 ; Irene Franco, Sara Lusini & Andrés Saab. Romance languages and Linguistic Theory, John Benjamins Publishing Company, pp.135-154, 2010 (2010)
BASE
Show details
19
What makes pair list answers available: An experimental approach
In: NELS 41 Proceedings ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00929986 ; NELS 41 Proceedings, Oct 2010, University of Pennsylvania, United States (2010)
BASE
Show details
20
Nominal constituents in French lexifier creoles - Probing the structuring role of grammaticalization
In: ISSN: 0920-9034 ; Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01121620 ; Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages, John Benjamins Publishing, 2007, 22, pp.Pages : 263-308 (2007)
BASE
Show details

Page: 1 2

Catalogues
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
Bibliographies
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
24
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern