DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2 3 4 5 6
Hits 1 – 20 of 106

1
Research-simulating linguistic problems for the classroom
In: IULC Working Papers; Vol 17 No 3 (2017): Special Volume: Reissue of Innovations in Linguistic Education, Volume 2 ; 1524-2110 (2018)
BASE
Show details
2
The Routledge handbook of language and humor
BLLDB
UB Frankfurt Linguistik
Show details
3
Rethinking reference: Towards a holistic approach to linguistic reference
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2016)
BASE
Show details
4
The Importance of Nouns in Text Processing
In: Stuart, Lauren M.; Taylor, Julia M.; & Raskin, Victor. (2013). The Importance of Nouns in Text Processing. Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 35(35). Retrieved from: http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/4sj6j35s (2013)
BASE
Show details
5
Automatic identification of metaphoric utterances
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2013)
BASE
Show details
6
Computational evaluation of a noun-driven syntax representation
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2012)
BASE
Show details
7
The semantics and pragmatics of translating culture-bound references in film dubbing
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2012)
BASE
Show details
8
On Oring on GTVH
In: Humor. - Berlin ; Boston : de Gruyter Mouton 24 (2011) 2, 223-231
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
9
Semantic and pragmatic failure in translating literary texts: Translators' inconsistency and/or textual resistance
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2011)
BASE
Show details
10
Organizing the wastebasket: A semantic system for describing inter-clausal collocation
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2011)
BASE
Show details
11
A semantic analysis of quantification in English
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2011)
BASE
Show details
12
The position, weight, and content of the English subject and pre-subject constituents in expository writing: Interactions between constituent orders, information structure, and subjecthood properties
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2011)
Abstract: This study is concerned with clarity of writing at three levels of a written text: the clarity of each sentence, the cohesion of adjacent sentences, and the coherence of all sentences in the text taken together. Sentence-level clarity pertains to the clarity of the proposition expressed by it, cohesion pertains to the clarity of the connections between consecutive propositions, and coherence pertains to the clarity of the unified message that all the propositions convey together. Ways to achieve these three levels of clarity are described by Williams' (2005), which follow principles of sentence structure known from three areas of linguistic studies: (i) four principles of word order, or more precisely, constituent order, (ii) information structure, which is the source of two of the four constituent ordering principles, and (iii) properties of prototypical subjects. First, the four constituent orders are (i) the canonical SVX order of English, (ii) the light-heavy constituent order, (iii) the given-new information order, and (iv) the topic-comment order. Second, information structure pertains to three pairs of concepts: (i) given and new information, (ii) topic and comment, and (iii) presupposition and focus. Each of these three pairs is conceptually distinct from the other two, although the three are often conflated in the literature. Lastly, the referents of prototypical subjects are (i) animate, (ii) agents, if the proposition involves actions that require an agent, and (iii) topics of propositions. Given this framework, Williams' guidelines for clear writing can be expressed as follows: (i) Individual sentences are clear when (a) their subject refers to the main participant in the proposition, preferably an animate participant and an agent where applicable, and the verb expresses the action described in the proposition, (b) the subject is light and is the first or an early constituent of the sentence, and (c) the sentence begins with given information and ends with new information; (ii) a sequence of sentences is cohesive when each sentence begins with information given at the end of the preceding sentence; and (iii) a passage is coherent when the topics of its sentences are closely related, expressed as subjects, and placed close to the beginning of the sentences. The above guidelines taken together fall short on many accounts because they do not take into account cases where two or more sentence-structuring principles conflict with each other. In order to address these gaps, this study examined to what extent sentence structures in good writing follow Williams' clarity principles. The study corpus consisted of seminal articles on composition theory written by ten leading professionals in the field, all presumably good writers. Given that the prototypical subject is the topic and/or an animate agent and that the subject is the first constituent in the canonical order, this study focused on subjects in the corpus to determine if they satisfied the four constituent orders: the canonical, light-heavy, given-new, topic-comment orders, i.e. if the subject was the first constituent, light, given, and the topic, and additionally, if it was animate and agentive. The study found that almost three quarters of the subjects in the corpus were in canonical position and contained given information. Among all referential subjects, about 82% were topics, almost half were animate, and about 37% were agents, indicating that topicality was the most predominant property of subjects. Most of the subjects (97.8%) were lighter than their predicates, and four of the five inverted subjects found were heavier than the inverted complements, as they should be. In whole-complement inversions, the subject was the focus, not the topic. Pre-subject (preposed or inverted) constituents obviously violate the canonical order, but most of them in the corpus also violated both the light-heavy and given-new orders. More than two thirds of pre-subject constituents were heavier than the subject, more than half of them expressed new information, and almost two-thirds did not contain more given information than the subject. Among the sentences that violated one or more structuring constraints in favor of others, there was no predominant pattern as to which constraints were stronger; the strength of the constraints differed from case to case. The results of the study have implications for both writing instruction and for theories of information structure with regard to expository writing.
Keyword: Linguistics|Rhetoric
URL: https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/dissertations/AAI3507250
BASE
Hide details
13
Semio-pragmatic analysis of cartoons discourse: A step towards semiotranslation
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2011)
BASE
Show details
14
Humor research
Raskin, Victor (Hrsg.); Ruch, Willibald (Hrsg.). - Berlin, New York : de Gruyter Mouton, 2010
IDS Bibliografie zur Gesprächsforschung
15
Towards a computational model of metaphor
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2010)
BASE
Show details
16
The primer of humor research
Raskin, Victor (Hrsg.). - Berlin / New York : Mouton de Gruyter, 2009
IDS Bibliografie zur Gesprächsforschung
Show details
17
How to understand and assess a theory: the evolution of the SSTH into the GTVH and now into the OSTH
In: Journal of literary theory. - Berlin : de Gruyter 3 (2009) 2, 285-311
BLLDB
Show details
18
Scripts for metaphor
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2009)
BASE
Show details
19
Issues in lexical acquisition of the Ukrainian language
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2009)
BASE
Show details
20
Participation roles in free indirect discourse
In: Theses and Dissertations Available from ProQuest (2009)
BASE
Show details

Page: 1 2 3 4 5 6

Catalogues
10
5
7
0
2
0
1
Bibliographies
38
0
0
8
0
0
0
0
3
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
44
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern