DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2
Hits 1 – 20 of 32

1
How do clinicians judge fluency in aphasia? (Gordon & Clough, 2022) ...
Gordon, Jean K.; Clough, Sharice. - : ASHA journals, 2022
BASE
Show details
2
How do clinicians judge fluency in aphasia? (Gordon & Clough, 2022) ...
Gordon, Jean K.; Clough, Sharice. - : ASHA journals, 2022
BASE
Show details
3
Factor analysis of spontaneous speech in aphasia (Gordon, 2020) ...
Gordon, Jean K.. - : ASHA journals, 2020
BASE
Show details
4
Factor analysis of spontaneous speech in aphasia (Gordon, 2020) ...
Gordon, Jean K.. - : ASHA journals, 2020
BASE
Show details
5
Lexical decision in aging: Stimulus variables and response time components ...
Kurczek, Jake; Gordon, Jean K. - : figshare, 2016
BASE
Show details
6
Lexical decision in aging: Stimulus variables and response time components ...
Kurczek, Jake; Gordon, Jean K. - : figshare, 2016
BASE
Show details
7
Semantic interference in a randomized naming task: Effects of age, order, and category
In: Cognitive neuropsychology. - Abingdon : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 30 (2014) 7, 476-494
OLC Linguistik
Show details
8
Semantic interference in a randomized naming task: Effects of age, order, and category
BASE
Show details
9
Judging Communicative Competence: Investigating Age-Related Stereotypes in Speech-Language Pathology Students
Abstract: The proportion of the US population over age 65 is projected to reach almost 80 million by the year 2040, doubling the numbers from 2000 (Administration on Aging, 2012). With the aging of the population, the incidence of age-related diseases and disorders like stroke and dementia is expected to increase, adding to the caseloads of speech-language pathologists (SLPs). Most SLPs, by contrast, are younger adults; over a quarter of SLPs in the US are under age 35 (ASHA, 2012). Thus, as the elderly population grows, more intergenerational communication encounters will occur between SLPs and their aging clients, increasing demands for cultural competence, specifically with regard to ageism. However, the field of speech-language pathology has seen little research into the impact of age-related stereotypes on service delivery (Armstrong & McKechnie, 2003). One’s interactions with people are implicitly shaped by stereotypes, widely held unconscious representations of groups of people (Devine, 1989). According to the Age Stereotypes in Interaction model (Hummert, 2012), there are three main factors that trigger stereotypes: the perceiver’s self-system, the context of the interaction, and physical traits. ‘Self-system’ refers to one’s beliefs and attitudes, which are themselves determined by one’s age, cognitive complexity, and past experiences (Hummert, 2012; Ryan, 2007). Stereotypes can be reinforced by the context in which intergenerational encounters occur. To illustrate, Hummert and colleagues (1998) found that younger adults used different language when speaking to older adults in the hospital vs an apartment. Aspects of physical appearance (e.g. grey hair, stooped posture) create an immediate impression of the older individual (Adams et al., 2012). Using photographs, Hummert and colleagues (1997) found that adults perceived to be older were stereotyped more negatively than younger-looking adults. Negative stereotypes may, in turn, affect older adult’s responses, resulting in a cycle of reinforced stereotypes and negative interactions (Ryan, 2007). Williams and colleagues (2009) found that nurses who used ‘elderspeak’ met with more resistance to care in their patients with dementia. To prevent such negative interactions, SLPs must become aware of the potential impact of implicit age-related stereotypes. The purpose of this study was to determine whether SLP students are influenced by age-related stereotypes when judging the communication of older adults.
URL: http://aphasiology.pitt.edu/2547/1/493-892-1-RV%28Gordon-Cheimariou-Taylor%29.pdf
BASE
Hide details
10
Semantic interference in a randomized naming task: Effects of age, order, and category
In: Cognitive neuropsychology. - Abingdon : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 30 (2013) 7, 476-494
OLC Linguistik
Show details
11
The Aging Neighborhood: Phonological Density in Naming
BASE
Show details
12
Word retrieval in ageing: an exploration of the task constraint hypothesis
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 25 (2011) 6-7, 774-788
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
13
Neighborhood effects in aging and aphasia
Gordon, Jean K.. - 2011
BASE
Show details
14
Word retrieval in aging: An exploration of task effects
Gordon, Jean K.. - 2010
BASE
Show details
15
Measuring the lexical semantics of picture description in aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 22 (2008) 7-8, 839-852
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
16
Measuring the lexical semantics of picture description in aphasia
Gordon, Jean K.. - 2008
BASE
Show details
17
Understanding paragrammatism: A comparative case study
BASE
Show details
18
A contextual approach to facilitating word retrieval in agrammatic aphasia
In: Aphasiology. - London [u.a.] : Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group 21 (2007) 6-8, 643-657
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details
19
Interpreting speech errors in aphasia
In: Brain & language. - Orlando, Fla. [u.a.] : Elsevier 103 (2007) 1-2, 122
OLC Linguistik
Show details
20
A neural signature of phonological access: distinguishing the effects of word frequency from familiarity and length in overt picture naming
In: Journal of cognitive neuroscience. - Cambridge, Mass. : MIT Press Journals 19 (2007) 4, 617-631
BLLDB
OLC Linguistik
Show details

Page: 1 2

Catalogues
0
0
13
0
0
0
0
Bibliographies
9
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
15
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern