DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2 3 4 5...31
Hits 1 – 20 of 601

1
THE EFFECTIVENESS OF USING DIGITAL STORYTELLING TECHNIQUE IN MULTICULTURAL CLASSROOMS IN ORDER TO RAISE AWARENESS OF TRANSNATIONALISM ...
BASE
Show details
2
THE EFFECTIVENESS OF USING DIGITAL STORYTELLING TECHNIQUE IN MULTICULTURAL CLASSROOMS IN ORDER TO RAISE AWARENESS OF TRANSNATIONALISM ...
BASE
Show details
3
Influence of Orthography in teaching English and second language acquisition ...
BASE
Show details
4
Influence of Orthography in teaching English and second language acquisition ...
BASE
Show details
5
Viewing angle in novice L2 lexical learning in British Sign Language (BSL) ...
Watkins, Freya. - : Open Science Framework, 2022
BASE
Show details
6
Spanish pragmatic markers' usage patterns in second language and heritage speakers
Abstract: This study examines the patterns of use of pragmatic markers in heritage speakers and second language (L2) learners of Spanish at different proficiency levels (advanced vs. low-intermediate) by comparing their performances in a series of oral tasks. The investigation is guided by two main goals: 1) to analyze whether these groups display the same patterns of pragmatic markers’ usage in terms of frequency, variety of markers and range of functions; 2) to examine how the variables of proficiency, speech rate, language use, and participation in immersion programs affect their use of pragmatic markers. Based on previous research, I hypothesized that heritage speakers would outperform L2 learners of Spanish, therefore, they would use pragmatic markers more frequently, as well as a broader range of pragmatic markers, and for a wider variety of functions in their oral discourse (Fernández et al., 2014; Montrul, 2008, 2011; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008; Said-Mohand, 2006; Sánchez-Muñoz, 2007). Additionally, I hypothesized the creation of an Intercultural Style (Blum Kulka, 1991; Kasper & Blum-Kulka, 1993) by the bilingual groups. This style is characterized by a) cross-linguistic transfer from the L1 to the L2 (including examples of code-switching and/or borrowings, as well as the transfer of certain discourse-pragmatic functions from the English pragmatic markers to the Spanish ones); b) the overuse of certain pragmatic markers; c) instances of uses of pragmatic markers that diverge from the “monolingual standardized norm” (Aijmer, 2011; Bou, Garcés & Gregori, Granger & Tyson, 1996; Cenoz, 2003; Gilquin, 2008; Nogueira da Silva, 2011; Said-Mohand, 2006; Sánchez-Muñoz, 2007; Sankoff et al., 1997; Thomas, 1983; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008). Regarding the second goal, I hypothesized that the variables of proficiency, speech rate, language use, and immersion would play a crucial role in the use of pragmatic markers for both bilingual groups. Regarding proficiency, I expected more proficient groups (advanced heritage speakers and L2 learners) to outperform their less proficient counterparts (low-intermediate) (Fernández et al., 2014; Torres, 2002; Torres & Potowski, 2008; Said-Mohand, 2006). Given the fact that naturalistic contexts and direct contact with native speakers of the target language favor the acquisition of pragmatic markers (Hellermann & Vergun, 2007; Polat, 2011; Sankoff et al., 1997), both the use of Spanish, and participation in immersion programs were also predicted to be positively correlated with the production of pragmatic expressions. A total of 77 participants took part in the experiment: 25 heritage speakers (13 advanced and 12 low-intermediate), 32 L2 learners (8 advanced and 24 low-intermediate), and 20 monolingually-raised native speakers of Spanish as a control group (10 speakers of Mexican Spanish and 10 of the Peninsular variety). Participants completed an oral task designed to elicit oral speech samples via a computer. Prompts included different contexts with open-ended questions (e.g., describe your best friend) and more interactive contexts (e.g., the performance of speech acts such as apologizing to a friend). A total of 539 oral samples (77 participants x 7 prompts) were transcribed and coded for pragmatic markers. Results from statistical analyses (one-way between-subjects ANOVA and Tukey HSD post-hoc analyses conducted in R) showed that all bilingual groups produced similar rates of pragmatic markers. In terms of the variety of expressions employed by participants, statistical analyses revealed significant differences between the low-intermediate L2 group and the rest of the bilingual groups, as well as between the advanced L2 learners and the low-intermediate HSs. No significant differences were found, however, between both heritage speakers’ groups nor between the heritage speakers and the advanced L2 group. Concerning the most frequent pragmatic expressions, there were similar patterns across all groups. For instance, ‘y’, ‘pero’, and ‘como’ were among the five most frequent pragmatic markers for all groups (including monolingually-raised native speakers). On the other hand, there were also differences between L2 learners and heritage speakers. For example, heritage speakers and monolinguals favored the use of ‘este’ and ‘pues’, whereas L2 learners preferred the use of ‘entonces’ and ‘sí’. The oral speech of both heritage speakers and L2 learners showed features that differentiated them from the monolingually-raised groups. Their Intercultural Style was characterized by the use of English pragmatic markers (e.g., ‘so’, ‘like’); higher frequencies of use of markers such as ‘como’, ‘también’, or ‘sí’; the overuse of certain markers in the speech of low-intermediate L2 learners (e.g., ‘y’, ‘pero’); as well as uses of pragmatic markers in ways that diverged from the “norm” of monolingual varieties (e.g., ‘también no’, ‘unfortunadamente’, etc.). Regarding the effect of proficiency, speech rate, use of language, and immersion on the patterns of pragmatic markers’ usage, a series of multiple regression analyses showed that L2 learners and heritage speakers are affected by them differently. For L2 learners, speech rate was significantly correlated with higher frequencies of pragmatic markers’ use. Concerning the range of pragmatic expressions used, proficiency, immersion, and speech rate had a significant effect. In contrast, the variable of use of Spanish was not a significant predictor. For heritage speakers, the analysis only revealed a significant correlation between the variable use of Spanish and the variety in pragmatic markers’ usage. In other words, those heritage speakers who reported using Spanish more frequently showed a broader range of pragmatic expressions. In contrast to L2 learners, proficiency, speech rate and participation in immersion experiences did not affect heritage speakers’ production of pragmatic markers. These findings suggest that the acquisition and use of pragmatic markers by heritage speakers and L2 learners are influenced by different variables, as well as shaped differently by their language practices. For L2 learners, proficiency, speech rate, and immersion experiences are significant predictors, but not for heritage speakers. In light of these results, I discuss how standardized tests traditionally used to measure proficiency do not reflect heritage speakers’ real oral command of Spanish, and how heritage speakers benefit from earlier and more naturalistic exposure to Spanish (vs. L2 learners). ; Limited ; Author requested closed access (OA after 2yrs) in Vireo ETD system
Keyword: Bilingualism; Heritage Language Acquisition; Heritage Speakers; L2 Learners; L2 Pragmatics; Pragmatic Markers; Pragmatics; Second Language Acquisition
URL: http://hdl.handle.net/2142/108311
BASE
Hide details
7
The perception, production and the effect of explicit instruction in L2 English and Spanish vowels in foreign language (FL) adult learners
BASE
Show details
8
Derrière la langue, les positionnements sociaux. Pouvoir faire entendre sa voix en langue seconde
In: Nouvelle Revue Synergies Canada; No. 15 (2022): La notion de « voix » en sociolinguistique et sciences sociales ; 2292-2261 (2022)
BASE
Show details
9
Investigating a shift in instructional approach in second language listening pedagogy at a university-based intensive English program
BASE
Show details
10
Additive Linking in L2 French Discourse by German Learners: Syntactic Embedding and Intonation Patterns
In: EISSN: 2226-471X ; Languages ; https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-03252581 ; Languages, MDPI, 2021, 6 (1), pp.20. ⟨10.3390/languages6010020⟩ (2021)
BASE
Show details
11
Additive Linking in L2 French Discourse by German Learners: Syntactic Embedding and Intonation Patterns
In: EISSN: 2226-471X ; Languages ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03312978 ; Languages, MDPI, 2021, 6 (1), pp.20. ⟨10.3390/languages6010020⟩ (2021)
BASE
Show details
12
Transcription et annotation de données orales pour étudier la prosodie en FLE : enjeux méthodologiques
In: Superare l'evanescenza del parlato. Un vademecum per il trattamento digitale di dati ; https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-03252621 ; Giuliano Bernini; Ada Valentini; Lorenzo Spreafico; Jacopo Saturno. Superare l'evanescenza del parlato. Un vademecum per il trattamento digitale di dati, Bergamo : Sestante, pp.167-199, 2021 (2021)
BASE
Show details
13
Un outil d'exploration de corpus d'apprenants pour la didactique des langues secondes
In: Journées d'études « De l'acquisition à la didactique (et vice-versa) » ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03418871 ; Journées d'études « De l'acquisition à la didactique (et vice-versa) », Nov 2021, Paris, France (2021)
BASE
Show details
14
Exploration outillée pour un corpus de productions orales des apprenants débutants en L2
In: Colloque "Influence translinguisitique : où en est-on aujourd'hui ?" ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03471025 ; Colloque "Influence translinguisitique : où en est-on aujourd'hui ?", Jul 2021, Toulouse, France (2021)
BASE
Show details
15
FEATURES AND INSTRUCTIONAL ISSUES OF LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AT THE LINGUISTIC UNIVERSITIES ...
Shamuratova, Dilnoza Jamolovna. - : Academic research in educational sciences, 2021
BASE
Show details
16
THE IMPORTANCE OF INSTRUMENTAL AND INTEGRATIVE MOTIVATION IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION ...
Mamajonova, Manzura Nematjon Kizi. - : Academic research in educational sciences, 2021
BASE
Show details
17
INSTRUMENTAL AND INTEGRATIVE MOTIVATIONS IN EFL CONTEXT: IN THE CASE OF TWO UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS OF TASHKENT STATE INSTITUTE OF ORIENTAL STUDIES ...
Yokubov Ulugbek. - : Zenodo, 2021
BASE
Show details
18
INSTRUMENTAL AND INTEGRATIVE MOTIVATIONS IN EFL CONTEXT: IN THE CASE OF TWO UNDERGRADUATE STUDENTS OF TASHKENT STATE INSTITUTE OF ORIENTAL STUDIES ...
Yokubov Ulugbek. - : Zenodo, 2021
BASE
Show details
19
Corpora and L2 acquisition ; the L1 Portuguese – L2 Spanish subcorpus of CEDEL2
BASE
Show details
20
Assessing the double phonemic boundary in the very initial stage of L3 acquisition ...
Parrish, Kyle. - : Open Science Framework, 2021
BASE
Show details

Page: 1 2 3 4 5...31

Catalogues
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
Bibliographies
4
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
597
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern