DE eng

Search in the Catalogues and Directories

Page: 1 2 3 4 5 6...10
Hits 21 – 40 of 183

21
How Is Information Integrated Across Fixations in Reading?
BASE
Show details
22
The Role of Words in Chinese Reading
BASE
Show details
23
Variation and Repetition in the Spelling of Young Children
BASE
Show details
24
How children learn to write words
Treiman, Rebecca; Kessler, Brett. - Oxford : Oxford University Press, 2014
MPI für Psycholinguistik
Show details
25
Letter knowledge in parent–child conversations
In: Reading and writing. - New York, NY : Springer Science+Business Media 27 (2014) 3, 407-429
OLC Linguistik
Show details
26
How children learn to write words
Treiman, Rebecca; Kessler, Brett. - Oxford [u.a.] : Oxford Univ. Press, 2014
BLLDB
UB Frankfurt Linguistik
Show details
27
Does graphotactic knowledge influence the learning of new spellings presented in isolation?
In: ISSN: 0922-4777 ; EISSN: 1573-0905 ; Reading and Writing ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03158216 ; Reading and Writing, Springer Verlag, 2014, 4 (2014)
BASE
Show details
28
Learning to Spell from Reading: General Knowledge about Spelling Patterns Influences Memory for Specific Words
In: ISSN: 1747-0218 ; EISSN: 1747-0226 ; Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology ; https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03158128 ; Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Taylor & Francis (Routledge), 2014, 67 (5), pp.1019-1036. ⟨10.1080/17470218.2013.846392⟩ (2014)
BASE
Show details
29
Young children's knowledge about the spatial layout of writing
BASE
Show details
30
Learning to read and spell words in different writing systems
Caravolas, Markéta; Samara, Anna. - : Oxford University Press, 2014
BASE
Show details
31
Syllabification of American English: Evidence from a Large-scale Experiment. Part Isup/sup
In: Journal of quantitative linguistics. - London : Routledge 20 (2013) 1, 45-67
OLC Linguistik
Show details
32
Syllabification of American English: Evidence from a Large-scale Experiment. Part II
In: Journal of quantitative linguistics. - London : Routledge 20 (2013) 2, 75-93
OLC Linguistik
Show details
33
Do reading habits influence aesthetic preferences?
In: Reading and writing. - New York, NY : Springer Science+Business Media 26 (2013) 8, 1381-1386
OLC Linguistik
Show details
34
Instruction matters: spelling of vowels by children in England and the US
In: Reading and writing. - New York, NY : Springer Science+Business Media 26 (2013) 3, 473-487
OLC Linguistik
Show details
35
Learning to Use an Alphabetic Writing System
In: Language learning and development. - Philadelphia, Pa. : Taylor & Francis 9 (2013) 4, 317-330
OLC Linguistik
Show details
36
Language comprehension and production
In: Experimental psycholgy (Hoboken, New Jersey, 2013), p. 523-547
MPI für Psycholinguistik
Show details
37
Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
In: ISSN: 1664-1078 ; Frontiers in Psychology ; https://hal.univ-lille.fr/hal-03613289 ; Frontiers in Psychology, 2013, Frontiers in Psychology, 4, ⟨10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696⟩ (2013)
BASE
Show details
38
How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings?
Pacton, Sébastien; Sobaco, Amélie; Fayol, Michel. - : Frontiers Media S.A., 2013
BASE
Show details
39
Do young children spell words syllabically? Evidence from learners of Brazilian Portuguese
BASE
Show details
40
Children benefit from morphological relatedness when they learn to spell new words
Abstract: Use of morphologically related words often helps in selecting among spellings of sounds in French. For instance, final /wa/ may be spelled oi (e.g., envoi “sendoff”), oit (e.g., exploit “exploit”), ois (e.g., siamois, “siamese”), or oie (e.g., joie “joy”). The morphologically complex word exploiter “to exploit”, with a pronounced t, can be used to indicate that the stem exploit is spelled with a silent t. We asked whether 8-year-old children benefited from such cues to learn new spellings. Children read silently stories which included two target nonwords, one presented in an opaque condition and the other in a morphological condition. In the opaque condition, the sentence provided semantic information (e.g., a vensois is a musical instrument) but no morphological information that could justify the spelling of the target word's final sound. Such justification was available in the morphological condition (e.g., the vensoisist plays the vensois instrument, which justifies that vensois includes a final silent s). 30 min after having read the stories, children's orthographic learning was assessed by asking them to choose the correct spelling of each nonword from among three phonologically plausible alternatives (e.g., vensois, vensoit, vensoie). Children chose correct spellings more often in the morphological condition than the opaque condition, even though the root (vensois) had been presented equally often in both conditions. That is, children benefited from information about the spelling of the morphologically complex word to learn the spelling of the stem.
Keyword: Psychology
URL: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3790073
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00696
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24109464
BASE
Hide details

Page: 1 2 3 4 5 6...10

Catalogues
5
0
46
0
3
0
0
Bibliographies
99
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
15
Linked Open Data catalogues
0
Online resources
0
0
0
0
Open access documents
33
0
0
0
0
© 2013 - 2024 Lin|gu|is|tik | Imprint | Privacy Policy | Datenschutzeinstellungen ändern